THE BIOLOGICAL BASES OF ADHD –

 Normal development

 Normal development of circuits involved with cognitive control

 ADHD a problem of control

 Hypotheses and Research

  References : Casey, Durston, and Fosella (2001). Evidence for a mechanistic model of cognitive control. Clinical Neuroscience Research 1, 267-282.

 

** I was unable to insert the tables from the PowerPoint into this word document; however, the descriptions of the tables are in this document.  I will try to get tables asap.

 

 

Information about the developing human brain is relatively sketchy despite the significant gains in the fields of pediatric neuroimaging and developmental neurobiology.

This is due to low mortality rates across the age range in addition to the rare occurrence of autopsies on this population.

 

A significant amount of brain development occurs in utero, but changes continue postnatally.

By 2 years, the brain has reached close to 80% of its adult weight.

This is also a period of rapid synapse formation that begins well before birth in nonhuman primates.

Based on limited human post-mortem data, it has been suggested that synaptogenesis may not occur concurrently but synaptic density may instead peak earlier in the auditory cortex, at 3 months, and alter in the middle frontal gyrus, at 15 months.

 

In both human and nonhuman primate studies the early synaptic density peaks are followed by a plateau phase that decreases during childhood and into adulthood.

PET studies of glucose metabolism suggest that maturation of local metabolic rates parallel the time course of overproduction and subsequent pruning of synapses.

 

The most consistently reported findings from these studies are: 1) a lack of any significant change in cerebral volume after 5 years of age; 2) a significant decrease in cortical gray matter by 12 years in parietal and prefrontal cortex; and 3) an increase in cerebral white matter throughout childhood and young adulthood. Some subcortical gray regions such as the basal ganglia have been reported to decrease during childhood, particularly in males. This decrease in basal ganglia volume (caudate nuclei and globus pallidum) has been shown to occur between roughly 6 and 12 years of age.

Cortical gray matter in the frontal and parietal cortices appears to decrease during childhood until roughly puberty.

White matter volume appears to increase throughout childhood and well into adulthood and these increases may be regional in nature. Reiss et al. [117] showed an increase in white matter in dorsal prefrontal cortex, but no in more ventral prefrontal regions across childhood. Sowell et al. [123] reported a significant change in frontostriatal regions between childhood and adulthood above and beyond other brain regions. The changes were observed in both dorsal and medial portions of prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia.

Connectively within these circuits, particularly between prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia is an important aspect of brain development to assess. Refinement of these projections may correspond to increase cognitive control over actions. Klingber et al. [127] showed anisotropy was lower in frontal white matter in children 8-12 years old compared with healthy adults.

They showed delayed myelination of the frontal lobes consistent with previous reports.

 

The available data on human brain development suggest protracted development of the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia.

Development of cognitive control appears to parallel this brain development.

 

Each of the basal ganglia thalamocortical circuits controls or supports a different set of behaviors that range from skeletal and eye movements to cognitive and emotionally driven actions as illustrated in Fig. 1.

The sensorimotor circuits (motor and oculomotor) subserve voluntary skeletal and eye movement control.

The circuits involving association cortex (dorsolateral and lateral orbital frontal cortex) are involved presumably in guided or planned actions.

The limbic circuit is thought to be involved in the approach or avoidance of emotionally relevant events.

 

The basal ganglia then act broadly to inhibit competing movements that would otherwise interfere with the desired action.

 

 

 

Figure 1 Basic circuitry of the basal ganglia thalamocortical circuit. On the left, the frontal cortex projects to the basal ganglia, then thalamus and the loop is closed with a projection back to the frontal cortex. On the right, this circuitry is better delineated. The frontal cortex projects to different areas of the striatum (i.e.., putamen or caudate nuclei) and then projects to either the direct or the indirect pathway. The direct pathway involves an inhibitory projection to the internal capsule of the globus pallidus (GPi) and substantia nigra (SNr) resulting in the dampening of an inhibitory projection to the thalamus which results in disinhibition of the  thalamus. The indirect pathway consists of an inhibitory projection to the external capsule of the globus pallidus (GPe) which dampens the inhibitory projection to the subthalamic nuclei (STN) resulting in excitation of the internal capsule of the globus pallidus and substantia nigra. This in turn leads to an inhibition of the thalamus.

 

Fig. 1. Simplified diagram of five parallel basal ganglia thalamocortical circuits including (1) the motor circuit with projection zones in the premotor cortex (PMC), supplementary motor areas (SMA), and primary motor cortex; (2) the oculomotor circuit with primary projection zones in the supplementary eye fields (SEF) and frontal eye fields (FEF); (3) dorsolateral prefrontal (DLPFC) circuit; (4) lateral orbital frontal (LOFC) circuit; (5) the limbic circuit with primary projection zones in the anterior cingulate (AC) and medial orbitofrontal cortex.

 

Assuming that the direct pathway is involved in facilitating cortically mediated behaviors, then its disruption may result in constantly interrupted behaviors such as those observed in ADHD or constantly interrupted thoughts such as those observed in schizophrenia.

 

If the indirect pathway is involved in inhibiting cortically mediated behavior, then its disruption may result in

irrepressible repetitive behaviors and thoughts similar to those observed in obsessive compulsive disorder and Tourette’s syndrome or in ruminations of hopelessness in depression.

 

Over-activity of the direct pathway may lead to irrepressible repetitive behaviors and over activity of the indirect pathway may lead to constantly interrupted behaviors.

The basal ganglia are involved in inhibition of behaviors [55] while the frontal cortex is involved in guiding these actions by supporting representations of relevant information from interference due to competing information.

 

The basal ganglia, which consist primarily of inhibitory projections (GABA), are involved in the inhibition of inappropriate behaviors and disruption in the development of this brain region results in deficits in inhibitory control.

 

One can hypothesize that the frontal cortex, which consists primarily of excitatory projections (glutamate) is involved in maintenance of relevant information for action and disruption of this brain region results in deficits in the ability to carry out the relevant actions.

 

Hypofrontality as observed in ADHD would result in constantly interrupted behaviors or actions.

 

Hyperfrontality as observed in obsessive compulsive disorder [44] would result in irrepressible behaviors.

 

These regions are strongly innervated by dopamine, a neurotransmitter thought to be critically involved in regulation of attention and behavior.

An important neuromodulator in the basal ganglia thalamocortical

circuitry is dopamine, particularly at the level of the prefrontal cortex and striatum.

 

Sustained attention tasks in rats are enhanced by administration of the DI dopamine receptor agonist.

 

Pharmacological studies have shown that amphetamine and methylphenidate, drugs that increase the amount of free dopamine at the synapse, lead to enhanced attention.

 

The effects of dopamine have been examined in several computational models and are predicted to be essential for the protection of stable representations [3] as well as in the prediction of reward.

 

Development of the dopaminergic system parallels the development of performance on tasks involving suppression of

attentional or behavior responses.

 

Performance on tasks of cognitive control show significant improvement with age during the first 12 years of life. One factor thought to contribute to this age-dependent increase in performance is the development of the dopaminergic system.

In humans, the development of this system is coincident with a period of rapid synaptogensis in dendritic spines followed bya slower plateau phase of growth until adolescence.  

 

In a second study of 26 children with and 26 children without ADHD, Casey et al, tested children on all three attention tasks. They  showed significant correlations between performance on these tasks and asymmetry and size of the basal ganglia (caudate nucleus and globus pallidus) and prefrontal cortex.

 

In contrast, behavioral data from the ADHD children typically was in the opposite direction or did not correlate.

 

These results imply that deficits in cognitive control observed in ADHD may be due to abnormalities of the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia and that variations in these structures in healthy individuals relates to performance on cognitive control tasks.

The following slides are based on a review by Baumeister and Hawkins (2001) in Clinical Neuropharmacology Vol 24, No 1, pp, 2-10

From its inception, ADHD has been thought to have a biologic etiology.

 

Structural Imaging Studies Follows:

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Location

ADHD probands        

Contrast subjects          

Findings              

Comments

Basal ganglia Aylwrd et al. 1996 [12]

10 boys

11 normal controls; 16 boys with ADHD + TS

Globus pallidus volume smaller in ADHD (significant on left)

Caudate and putamen also smaller in ADHD, also smaller in ADHD,

Castellanos et al 1996 [15]

55 healthy controls

55 healthy controls

Normal symmetry in prefrontal brain, caudate, and globus pallidus significantly decreased in ADHD; cerebellum volume also smaller

Data support hypothesis that prefrontal-striatal- cortical circuitry mediates ADHD, particularly on right side

Filipek et al. 1997 [13]

15 males (same subjects as Semrud-Clikeman et a. 1994)[77]

15 normal controls

Caudate smaller (only) significant left) in ADHD; right anterior superior white matter also significantly diminished; posterior white matter volumes decreased only in stimulant non-responder

First study to quantify gray and white matter separately; authors suggest using medication response to subtype patient groups

Mataro et al. 1997 [78]

11 adolescents with ADHD

19 healthy control subjects (three grils in each group\

Right caudate larger in ADHD; larger caudate nucleus areas associated with poorer performance on tests of attention and higher ratings on Conner Teachers Rating Scale

Single slice axial MRI-not volumetric measure; only study to find larger size in ADHD

Cerebellum Berquin et al. 1998 [18]

46 right-handed boys (subset of Castellanos 1996) [15]

47 right-handed boys

Posterior inferior cerebellar vermis volume and area significantly smaller

Contrast survived covariance for total cerebral volume differences

Mostofsky et al. 1998 [19]

12 males

23 males

Posterior inferior cerebellar vermis area significantly smaller

Contrast survived covariance for total cerebral volume differences

Castellanos et al. 2001 [16]

50 girls

50 girls

Posterior inferior cerebellar vermis volume significantly smaller (-12%, same as boys)

Contrast survived covariance for total cerebral volume; most robust and replicated finding in ADHD

 

Brain Size:  Three magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have reported that total cerebral volume was significantly lower (by approximately 5%) in children with ADHD compared with control subjects (24-26).

Moreover, in each study the ADHD group had a significantly lower mean intelligence quotient (IQ) than control subjects.

IQ was statistically controlled, no significant difference in cerebral volume was found (24,26).

In addition, two independently conducted MRI studies found no difference in brain volume using ADHD and control subjects that did not differ in IQ (27,28).

Thus, no difference between persons with ADHD and control subjects.

 

Frontal Lobe:  Filipek et al. (27) reported that total frontal lobe volume and right-side (but not left-side) frontal lobe volume were significantly decreased in children with ADHD.

In addition, the right side (R) greater than left side (R>L) asymmetry in frontal lobe volume that was observed in control subjects was significantly reduced in children with ADHD.

Another group of investigators (26) reported essentially the same pattern of results except that there was no significant difference between control subjects and children with ADHD in total anterior frontal volume.

In that study (26), the smaller volume of the right frontal region in ADHD was still observed after controlling statistically for IQ differences.

 

Consistent with these observations, another study (30) found that children with ADHD had a smaller right (but not left) frontal lobe width than control subjects and that children with ADHD had reduced R>L frontal width asymmetry.

Considering the consistency of these findings, it is somewhat surprising that an MRI study of children who developed ADHD secondarily to closed brain injury of frontal lobe lesions actually predicted the nonoccurrence of ADHD.

 

Basal Ganglia:    One study found that total volume of the caudate nucleus was reduced in children with ADHD compared with controls, whereas two studies found no effect.

Two studies (27,31) found that the left but not the right caudate was decreased in subjects with ADHD compared with control subjects, whereas two other studies [using overlapping samples [25,26]) found the opposite pattern of results.

Filipek et al. (27) reported that control subjects had a smaller right than left caudate nucleus volume, whereas in subjects with ADHD these volumes were equal (i.e., children with ADHD showed an absence of R<L asymmetry).

 

 

Hynd et al. (31), like Filipek et al. (27), found that the right caudate (using area measures in a single axial section) was smaller than the left in control subjects.

Aylward et al. (29) found no asymmetry in caudate volume in either children with ADHD or control children.

 

Corpus Callosum:

Two studies found that children with ADHD have reduced size of posterior regions of the corpus callosum only (28,34), two found that the abnormality is confined to the anterior corpus callosum (35,36), one found abnormalities in both anterior and posterior regions (37), and one found no morphologic abnormalities in the corpus callosum of subjects with ADHD compared with normal subjects who had siblings with ADHD (38).

 

Cerebellum:   One study (26) found that cerebellar volume (after controlling for total cerebral volume) was significantly decreased in children with ADHD compared with control subjects, but there were no differences between groups with respect to total midsagittal area of the vermian lobules.

Conversely, a follow-up study by the same group of investigators (24), which employed most of the same subjects with ADHD but used an improved image quantification technique, found the opposite pattern of effect.

Another study using computed tomography scans found evidence suggestive of mild cerebellar atrophy in adults with a history of hyperactivity (22).

However, in this study, group differences did not reach statistical significance and many of the subjects had histories of antisocial behavior and alcohol abuse.

 

Lateral Ventricles:

One MRI study (26) found that the left-side but not the right-side lateral ventricle was decreased in volume in children with ADHD. Another study (28) reported that posterior but not the anterior lateral ventricle volume was increased in children with ADHD.

 

Functional Imaging Studies:

Frontal Lobes:

Although most of these studies reported evidence for decreased activity of this brain area in subjects with ADHD, the evidence is not entirely consistent.

Of the studies that reported significant effects in frontal lobe activity (41-45); five found decreased activity exclusively, although activity was increased in some frontal loci 946-50); three reported no differences between subjects with ADHD and control subjects (51-53); and one reported that subjects with ADHD had increased frontal lobe activity (54).

 

Table 3 summarizes the functional abnormalities that have been reported in particular regions of the frontal lobes of subjects with ADHD.

 

 

Frontal area                               Result                           Reference

Anterior Frontal L                                                                             50

Anterior Frontal L                                                                     47

Anterior Frontal                                     *                                        47

Anteriormedial Frontal                          *                                         47

Dorsal Frontal                                                                          46

Global Frontal                                                                    42

Global Frontal                                                                    54

Global Frontal R                                                                             41

Global Frontal L                                  *                                              41

Global Frontal L                                                                        43

Inferior Prefrontal R                                                            48

Inferior Frontal Gyrus R                                                                 49

 

Frontal area                               Result                           Reference

Inferior Posterior Front                                                        50

Mesial Prefrontal R                                                             48

Middle Frontal Gyrus R                                                                  49

Posterior Frontal L                                                                 47

Posterior Frontal                    *                                         47

Precentral L                                                                        49

Prefrontal                                                                            46

Prefrontal R                                                                         44

Premotor                                                                            45

Premotor                                                                            49

Premotor R                                                                         48

Superior Posterior Front                                                        50

Superior Prefrontal                                                                45

 

 

Basal Ganglia:

Three studies (52, 53, 54) reported that striatal activity was significantly decreased in children with ADHD.  However, studies that examined specifically the caudate nucleus or the putamen provided little corroborating support. Of the six (42, 45, 47, 48, 50, 51) on caudate nucleus only one (48) found a significant difference.  In the same studies looking at the putamen, only one found differences (47). Decrease in females.

 

Occipital Lobes:

Few studies have been done looking at the Occipital lobes.  The results are inconsistent here also.  One (47) found decreased activity in ADHD girls but not boys, one study found increased activity in ADHD boys but not girls (50). One found no difference between ADHD and controls (46).

 

Temporal lobes:

Again results were inconsistent.  In a study (45) with adults there was decrease activity in the left but not the right anterior and posterior, but not the middle, temporal lobes. In a second study (50) (adolescent), there was a decrease in the right but not the left temporal regions of boys but not girls. In a third study there was an increase in the middle temporal lobe on the right side only. One study (46) found no effect.

 

Parietal lobes:

One study found decrease in both left and right (45).  In a second study there was decrease on the left side only in boys (50).  In a third study (47), there was significant increase on the right side only. One study (46) found no effect. 

 

Discussion:

There seems to be a consensus among experts today that ADHD is associated with structural and/or functional abnormalities in the brain.

Neuroimaging literature provides no convincing evidence for the existence of abnormality in the brains of persons with ADHD.

Few trends in the findings are apparent.

Structural imaging studies of the corpus callosum have consistently reported that areas of this structure are decreased in subjects with ADHD. However, studies are inconsistent with respect to the particular areas affected.  A possible explanation of the data is that lesions anywhere in the corpus callosum is associated with ADHD, but different parts of the corpus callosum are connected to different cortical areas so the effect should be variable.

Attention Deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHA) and childhood onset schizophrenia (COS)

 

ADHD is characterized by a slightly smaller (4%) total brain volume (both white and gray matter), less-consistent abnormalities of the basal ganglia and a striking (15%) decrease in posterior inferior cerebellar vermal volume. These changes do not progress with age. In contrast, patients with COS have smaller brain volume due to a 10% decrease in cortical gray volume.

In normal child development, there are robust and complex changes in white and gray matter.

White matter volume increases linearly during this age range, reflecting increasing myelination [4,9], while gray matter volume increases until early to mid-adolescence before decreasing during late adolescence [8], presumably from synaptic pruning and reduction of neuropil.

 

 

Abnormalities appear to be a fixed, rather than an ongoing, process. Longitudinal changes during childhood and adolescence did not differ between 73 ADHD subjects. These anatomic abnormalities are not due to stimulant drug effects since the 17 medication-naïve patients showed the same brain pattern at least before age 4. Impulsivity per se is not likely to cause the late progressive abnormalities seen for the schizophrenic group. Candidate processes for the abnormalities in ADHD focus on prenatal or early postnatal events.

 

Decreased total brain volume and increased lateral ventricular volume as seen in adult onset schizophrenia.

 

The decreased brain volume here is due exclusively to the robust 10% decrease in cortical gray matter, as the total white matter volume does not differ significantly between the COS and healthy groups.

 

Clinically these changes parallel a decline in full-scale IQ [29] and lack of normal maturation of neurological status [30] for these patients.

 

Clinically these changes parallel a decline in full-scale IQ [29] and lack of normal maturation of neurological status [30] for these patients.

 

The stress and abnormal thought and behavior experienced by psychotic patients might be responsible for the progressive loss of gray matter and cytoarchitectonic deficiencies found in these individuals.

Could schizophrenic behavior be a cause rather than an effect of the decreased and decreasing frontal gray brain volume?

Could sever inattention and impulsivity produce small gray matter volume?

Is at least consistent with the idea that changes in synaptic efficacy could play a critical role in the onset of schizophrenia [39,40]

 

Even the abnormality in environmental input to the brain of a schizophrenic patient is considerably less severe than the conditions involved with most of the experiments exploring plasticity

Schizophrenic behaviors result from these abnormalities rather than the reverse

 

The total and regional growth curves for this group run parallel to normal brain curves [21].

Thus, the events leading to the anatomic differences in ADHD probably occur early in neurodevelopment.

Even a subtle injury during this vulnerable process of neurodevelopment and organization in utero (second or third trimester) can affect the brain development and size globally, thus explaining the changes seen in ADHD [50].

 

Incidence of pregnancy-related complications, prematurity are slightly higher in ADHD [51].

Prematurity, leaves us without a specific hypothesis of what might mediate this subtle global reduction to brain volume

This region of cerebellar vermis is highly dopaminergic [52] and appears, like most brain volumetric measures to be highly heritable, [Giedd J, Castellanos X: unpublished data].

The posterior inferior verimis, thus, may be an important part of cerebello-striato-frontal circuitry and hence in the aetiopathogenesis of ADHD.

 

 

Thus any defect in dopaminergic circuitry could alter these growth modulators and ultimately the brain size and development.

These alleles have been examined in ADHD populations and show no significant association with any of these brain volumetric measures [55].

Subtle abnormalities in BDNF, NT--3 and others may cause localized abnormality of dopamine circuitry.

 

 

The caudate nucleus volume is decreased in boys with ADHD [56,57].

This is interesting as healthy girls have a larger caudate nucleus, probably owing to higher concentration of estrogen receptors in the region. This might explain the smaller caudate volume in ADHD boys and lower incidence of ADHD in girls. This would not account for the fact that brain MRI findings for ADHD boys and girls are very similar.

This reduction of the neuropil could result from reduction in the numbers of synaptic connections made between neurons. Normally a postnatal increase in synaptic connections followed by a decrease in synaptic connections that extends as far as mid-adolescence in some parts of the brain [61].

(A reduced number of neurons generated during early neurodevelopment) (An excessive degree of synapse reduction of an initially fairly normal number of synapses and neurons).

 

Post-mortem studies to determine if there is a reduced cell number in the brains of people with schizophrenia have not produced consistent results in many parts of the brain [34,60].

Found increased densities of neurons in the prefrontal cortex and temporal lobe [62,64].

Decreased loss of expression of synaptic markers in schizophrenia [65] and most recently decreased expression of several functional genes important in presynaptic function and development [66].

Reduced neuropil hypothesis.

 

As development progresses the extra connects are eliminated or pruned.

The possible mechanisms behind pathologic elimination of the synapses are too numerous and complex to thoroughly review here.

Neuromuscular junction (NMJ)

At birth in the rodent, all muscle fibers are innervated by at least two axons, and by 3 weeks postnatally all but one axon has been eliminated from all of the muscle cells [68].

Requires activation of the system because it fails to take place in paralyzed preparations.

 

 

To involve a number of protein kinases, enzymes which regulate protein function by altering their state of phosphorylation. Neurotransmitter receptors are known to be targets for kinase action leading to synapse elimination Activation of appropriate kinases, phosphorylation of neurotransmitter receptors and subsequent, selective destabilization of the synapses involving those receptors [69]. Loss of synaptic acetylcholine receptors has been shown to be an early step in the loss of synapses at the NMJ [70]. Differential activation of localized kinases, which have different effects on receptor stability.

 

(protein kinase C or PKC) in synapse elimination in the central nervous system has been obtained in experiments on the CF/PC system mentioned above.

Mutation of one isoform of PKC which inactivates the kinase has been shown to block synapse elimination in the mouse cerebellum [71]. The activity of this particular molecule, PKC, is essential for normal elimination of redundant synapses.

Trophic factors have been shown to have powerful effects on neuronal survival and synaptic structure and function during development [50, 72-74]. Competition for a limited supply of trophic material has been postulated to account for at least a portion of the synapse loss that occurs during development. Trophin BDNF has been shown to prevent the normally occurring pruning that is essential for development of the normal architecture of the visual cortex [75] and an inadequate supply could result in inadequate development or survival of cortical synapses.

Has been shown that a neuropeptide, vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP) controls the duration of the mitotic cycle in the neuroblasts in the ventricular germinal zone and that this affects the total number of neurons that get born during development. Result in markedly microcephalic animals [74]. Such interference with the process of neuron generation could be involved in the early deficit in brain size seen in ADHD.

Following on the initial speculation of Feinberg [76], McGlashan and Hoffman [35] and others have used computer modeling of neural networks to test the plausibility of the ‘over-pruning’ hypothesis of schizophrenia